vrijdag 16 december 2011

Van de Cruys & Wagemans - iPerception Putting reward in art: A tentative prediction error account of visual art

 

Kanaal: i-Perception
Gepost op: donderdag 15 december 2011 17:12
Auteur: Pion
Onderwerp: Putting reward in art: A tentative prediction error account of visual art

 

The predictive coding model is increasingly and fruitfully used to explain a wide range of findings in perception. Here we discuss the potential of this model in explaining the mechanisms underlying aesthetic experiences. Traditionally art appreciation has been associated with concepts such as harmony, perceptual fluency, and the so-called good Gestalt. We observe that more often than not great artworks blatantly violate these characteristics. Using the concept of prediction error from the predictive coding approach, we attempt to resolve this contradiction. We argue that artists often destroy predictions that they have first carefully built up in their viewers, and thus highlight the importance of negative affect in aesthetic experience. However, the viewer often succeeds in recovering the predictable pattern, sometimes on a different level. The ensuing rewarding effect is derived from this transition from a state of uncertainty to a state of increased predictability. We illustrate our account with several example paintings and with a discussion of art movements and individual differences in preference. On a more fundamental level, our theorizing leads us to consider the affective implications of prediction confirmation and violation. We compare our proposal to other influential theories on aesthetics and explore its advantages and limitations.


Artikel weergeven...

woensdag 14 december 2011

GERT website

Check our updated GERT website:

 

http://www.gestaltrevision.be/gert

 

It has the new GERT release and an updated manual.

It now also contains a nice “example stimuli” section (just follow the link).

 

Feel free to spread the word.

 

(thanks to Rudy for the smooth implementation)

vrijdag 9 december 2011

Prof. dr. Raymond van Ee bijzonder hoogleraar Entrepreneurship and innovatio...

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 

via Persberichten on 12/9/11

De heer prof. dr. Raymond van Ee is met ingang van 1 november 2011 benoemd tot bijzonder hoogleraar Entrepreneurship and innovation in life sciences aan de Faculteit Natuurwetenschappen, Wiskunde en Informatica van de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen.

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

woensdag 7 december 2011

Google Scholar Citations Open To All

Google has made it easy to track your publications and help others to do so. (Thanks Bart M for pointing this out)

 
 

Naudojant „Google Reader" atsiųsta jums nuo Jonas:

 
 

per Google Scholar Blog autorius Anurag 11.11.16

A few months ago, we introduced a limited release of Google Scholar Citations, a simple way for authors to compute their citation metrics and track them over time. Today, we're delighted to make this service available to everyone! Click here and follow the instructions to get started.

Here's how it works. You can quickly identify which articles are yours, by selecting one or more groups of articles that are computed statistically. Then, we collect citations to your articles, graph them over time, and compute your citation metrics - the widely used h-index; the i-10 index, which is simply the number of articles with at least ten citations; and, of course, the total number of citations to your articles. Each metric is computed over all citations and also over citations in articles published in the last five years.

Your citation metrics will update automatically as we find new citations to your articles on the web. You can also set up automated updates for the list of your articles, or you can choose to review the suggested updates. And you can, of course, manually update your profile by adding missing articles, fixing bibliographic errors, and merging duplicate entries.

As one would expect, you can search for profiles of colleagues, co-authors, or other researchers using their name, affiliation, or areas of interest, e.g., researchers at US universities or researchers interested in genomics. You can add links to your co-authors, if they already have a profile, or you can invite them to create one.

You can also make your profile public, e.g., Alex Verstak, Anurag Acharya. If you choose to make your profile public, it can appear in Google Scholar search results when someone searches for your name, e.g., [alex verstak]. This will make it easier for your colleagues worldwide to follow your work.

We would like to thank the participants in the limited release of Scholar Citations for their detailed feedback. They were generous with their time and patient with an early version. Their feedback greatly helped us improve the service. The key challenge was to make profile maintenance as hands-free as possible for those of you who prefer the convenience of automated updates, while providing as much flexibility as possible for those who prefer to curate their profile themselves.

Here is hoping that Google Scholar Citations will help researchers everywhere view and track the worldwide influence of their own and their colleagues' work.

Posted by: James Connor, Software Engineer

 
 

Veiksmai, kuriuos dabar galite atlikti:

 
 

maandag 5 december 2011

living books

A great collection of 'living books', some of them on cognitive science and a particularly interesting one on open science:

http://www.livingbooksaboutlife.org/


Cheers,
Sander


zaterdag 26 november 2011

"Bayesian confusions surrounding simplicity and likelihood in perceptual organization"

Abstract

In the study of perceptual organization, the Occamian simplicity
principle (which promotes efficiency) and the Helmholtzian likelihood
principle (which promotes veridicality) have been claimed to be
equivalent. Proposed models of these principles may well yield similar
outcomes (especially in everyday situations), but as argued here,
claims that the principles are equivalent confused subjective
probabilities (which are used in Bayesian models of the Occamian
simplicity principle) and objective probabilities (which are needed in
Bayesian models of the Helmholtzian likelihood principle).
Furthermore, Occamian counterparts of Bayesian priors and conditionals
have led to another confusion, which seems to have been triggered by a
dual role of regularity in perception. This confusion is discussed by
contrasting complete and incomplete Occamian approaches to perceptual
organization.

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0001691811001661

vrijdag 25 november 2011

Gestalt perception and the decline of global precedence in older subjects

Gestalt perception and the decline of global precedence in older subjects

Markus R. Staudinger a,b, Gereon R. Fink a,c, Clare E. Mackay d,e and Silke Lux a,* 

Our visual world is hierarchically organized. Hierarchical processing is frequently inves- tigated using Navon figures (large letters made up of smaller ones). In young adults, many studies reported faster reaction times (RT) to target letters presented at the global level [i.e., global precedence (GP)]. Furthermore, an age-related decline of this GP has been reported. We tested whether deficits in perceptual grouping via Gestalt laws (Gestalt principles of Proximity and Continuity) might contribute to this decline. In a directed attention task with valid and invalid cues, 20 young (mean age 22) and 20 older (mean age 57) male subjects had to indicate whether a target letter appeared at the global or local level of a Navon figure. The number of local letters forming the global figure was modulated in 5 steps. As expected, during valid trials, young adults showed a GP that linearly increased with increasing numbers of local letters (i.e., GP enhancement). This suggests that GP is related to perceptual grouping via Gestalt laws. By contrast, the group of older subjects demonstrated no precedence effect in RT and a non-significant trend toward GP in error rates (ER). No GP enhancement with an increasing number of local elements was observed. Exploratory analysis revealed that individual insensitivity to the modulation of matrix density, as revealed by a lack of global RT acceleration, was restricted to subjects that showed an overall local precedence (LP). Because older subjects tended to more frequently display an insensitivity to matrix modulation and an LP, we conclude that deficient Gestalt detection as indicated by non-enhanced global RT might contribute to the RT-related decline of GP with age. 

doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.08.001 
cortex 47 (2011) 854-862    

  
----
Krista Overvliet, PhD.
 
Laboratory of  Experimental Psychology
University of Leuven
Tiensestraat 102, bus 3711
Room 00.74
3000 Leuven
Belgium
 
phone: +3216326146
skype: kristaovervliet
krista.overvliet@gmail.com
krista.overvliet@ppw.kuleuven.be
http://web.me.com/krista.overvliet

woensdag 23 november 2011

The construction of perceptual grouping displays using GERT

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 


Abstract  
To study perceptual grouping processes, vision scientists often use stimuli consisting of spatially separated local elements that, together, elicit the percept of a global structure. We developed a set of methods for constructing such displays and implemented them in an open-source MATLAB toolbox, GERT (Grouping Elements Rendering Toolbox). The main purpose of GERT is to embed a contour in a field of randomly positioned elements, while avoiding the introduction of a local density cue. However, GERT's modular implementation enables the user to create a far greater variety of perceptual grouping displays, using these same methods. A generic rendering engine grants access to a variety of element-drawing functions, including Gabors, Gaussians, letters, and polygons.

  • Content Type Journal Article
  • Pages 1-8
  • DOI 10.3758/s13428-011-0167-8
  • Authors
    • Maarten Demeyer, Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven (K.U. Leuven), Tiensestraat 102, bus 3711, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
    • Bart Machilsen, Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, University of Leuven (K.U. Leuven), Tiensestraat 102, bus 3711, 3000 Leuven, Belgium

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

dinsdag 8 november 2011

Bayesian Inference: From Spikes to Behaviour

Confirmed Speakers:

Michael Black, Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
Daniel Braun, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
Opher Donchin, Ben Gurion University of Negev, Be'er Sheva, ISRAEL
Dominik Endres, Eberhard Karls University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
Adrienne Fairhall, University of Washington, Illinois, USA
Moritz Grosse-Wentrup, Max Planck Institute for Intelligent Systems, Tübingen, Germany
Peter Földiak, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, UK
Konrad Körding, University of Chicago, Illinois, USA
Peter Latham, University College London, London, UK
Laurence Maloney, University of New York, New York, USA
Aude Oliva, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Massachusetts, USA
Uta Noppeney, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany
Jan Peters, TU Darmstadt, Darmstadt, Germany
Josh Tenenbaum, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Massachusetts, USA


http://www.bccn-tuebingen.de/events/bernstein-symposium-series-2011/symposium-c.html 

What the Brain Sees After the Eye Stops Looking

 
 

Aan u verzonden door Sander via Google Reader:

 
 


When we gaze at a shape and then the shape disappears, a strange thing happens: We see an afterimage in the complementary color. Now a Japanese study has observed for the first time an equally strange illusion: The afterimage appears in a "complementary" shape—circles as hexagons, and vice-versa.

"The finding suggests that the afterimage is formed in the brain, not in the eye," the author, Hiroyuki Ito of Kyushu University, wrote in an email. More specifically, the illusion is produced in the brain's shape-processing visual cortex, not the eye's light-receiving, message-sending retina. The findings appear in an upcoming issue of Psychological Science, a journal published by the Association for Psychological Science.

Ito conducted three experiments with 82, 92, and 44 participants respectively. In the first two, he showed participants yellow circles or hexagons – outlined or filled, static or rotating on a gray ground. In each, after they observed the images for 10 seconds, the images disappeared, leaving only the blank gray field. The observers were asked to indicate which of seven shapes, on a piece of paper, the afterimage most resembled.

In the third experiment, Ito split the visual field between the two eyes. In the left eye, participants saw rotating circles and hexagons, as well as rotating asterisk-like "stars"—shapes that were neither round nor angular. The right eye viewed static circles in all conditions.  When the circles, hexagons, and stars disappeared, the left field was black, which suppressed the formation of afterimages, and the right was white, which heightened it.

In Experiments 1 and 2, participants tended to see circles after hexagons and hexagons after circles. In the third, the right eye produced the most angular afterimages when circles had been projected in the left eye; the most rounded ones after the rotating hexagons; and after the "stars," images that were neither circular nor edged.

How did Ito infer that the brain, not the eye, was producing these afterimages? He eliminated the theory that the afterimage was a manifestation of "retinal bleaching"—when the photoreceptors on the retina become ineffective or fatigued through prolonged exposure to light.  Viewing static circles or hexagons produce circular or hexagonal bleached areas on the retina. However, the afterimage shapes were not in the bleached shapes.  A spinning circle or hexagon produces a circular trace of light on the retina, causing circular shape of retinal bleaching just as painting on the retina. However, spinning circles produced hexagonal afterimages and vice versa.

Retinal bleaching could not produce "an afterimage shape different from the [typical] retinal bleaching shape." Neither could the retina transfer information taken in by the left eye to produce an afterimage in the right eye.  "The only site that can happen is the brain."

The research adds to science's understanding of the role of the brain in vision. "People tend to think that afterimages are meaningless by-products arising from the physiological characteristics of the eye," wrote Ito. "But I think that the afterimages reflect brain activities and provide us the means to know those activities in a directly visible form."


 
 

Dingen die u vanaf hier kunt doen:

 
 

vrijdag 28 oktober 2011

Fwd: CVNet - AVA Xmas Meeting abstract submission extension

Still time to submit an abstract for the AVA meeting! (York, UK)

__________________________________________________________________________


The AVA Christmas Meeting will be held at the University of York on 19
December 2011.

This year's organiser is Peter Thompson.

The conference website is at

http://www.theava.net/conf/index.php?conference=Meeting&schedConf=X2011

The original deadline for the submission of Abstracts was 1 November
2011. We have decided to extend the deadline to midnight on Sunday 6
November. Please submit abstracts via the AVA website and register for
the meeting at the same time.

WIth best wishes

Peter Thompson: Meeting Orgnaniser
Tom Troscianko: AVA Secretary

woensdag 26 oktober 2011

Illusory Contours over Pathological Retinal Scotomas.

 
 

Aan u verzonden door Sander via Google Reader:

 
 


PLoS One. 2011; 6(10): e26154
De Stefani E, Pinello L, Campana G, Mazzarolo M, Lo Giudice G, Casco C

Our visual percepts are not fully determined by physical stimulus inputs. Thus, in visual illusions such as the Kanizsa figure, inducers presented at the corners allow one to perceive the bounding contours of the figure in the absence of luminance-defined borders. We examined the discrimination of the curvature of these illusory contours that pass across retinal scotomas caused by macular degeneration. In contrast with previous studies with normal-sighted subjects that showed no perception of these illusory contours in the region of physiological scotomas at the optic nerve head, we demonstrated perfect discrimination of the curvature of the illusory contours over the pathological retinal scotoma. The illusion occurred despite the large scar around the macular lesion, strongly reducing discrimination of whether the inducer openings were acute or obtuse and suggesting that the coarse information in the inducers (low spatial frequency) sufficed. The result that subjective contours can pass through the pathological retinal scotoma suggests that the visual cortex, despite the loss of bottom-up input, can use low-spatial frequency information from the inducers to form a neural representation of new complex geometrical shapes inside the scotoma.


 
 
 
 

vrijdag 21 oktober 2011

Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure

My friend here is reinventing phrenology…

 

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22012980

 

Proc Biol Sci. 2011 Oct 19. [Epub ahead of print]

 

Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure.

Kanai R, Bahrami B, Roylance R, Rees G.

UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK.

 

Abstract

The increasing ubiquity of web-based social networking services is a striking feature of modern human society. The degree to which individuals participate in these networks varies substantially for reasons that are unclear. Here, we show a biological basis for such variability by demonstrating that quantitative variation in the number of friends an individual declares on a web-based social networking service reliably predicted grey matter density in the right superior temporal sulcus, left middle temporal gyrus and entorhinal cortex. Such regions have been previously implicated in social perception and associative memory, respectively. We further show that variability in the size of such online friendship networks was significantly correlated with the size of more intimate real-world social groups. However, the brain regions we identified were specifically associated with online social network size, whereas the grey matter density of the amygdala was correlated both with online and real-world social network sizes. Taken together, our findings demonstrate that the size of an individual's online social network is closely linked to focal brain structure implicated in social cognition.

 

 

Ervin Poljac, PhD

Laboratory of Experimental Psychology

University of Leuven (K.U. Leuven)

Tiensestraat 102

B-3000 Leuven

Belgium

tel. +32-16-32.61.43 (office)

fax. +32-16-32.60.99

Email: Ervin.Poljac@psy.kuleuven.be

 

Grouping and Emergent Features in Vision

Grouping and Emergent Features in Vision: Toward a Theory of Basic Gestalts

James R. Pomerantz , Mary C. Portillo1

Department of Psychology, Rice University

Received 14 May 2010; revised 17 March 2011; Accepted 27 March 2011. Available online 7 October 2011.

 

Gestalt phenomena are often so powerful that mere demonstrations can confirm their existence, but Gestalts have proven hard to define and measure. Here we outline a theory of basic Gestalts (TBG) that defines Gestalts as emergent features (EFs). The logic relies on discovering wholes that are more discriminable than are the parts from which they are built. These wholes contain EFs that can act as basic features in human vision. As context is added to a visual stimulus, a hierarchy of EFs appears. Starting with a single dot and adding a second yields the first two potential EFs: the proximity (distance) and orientation (angle) between the two dots. A third dot introduces two more potential EFs: symmetry and linearity; a fourth dot produces surroundedness. This hierarchy may extend to collinearity, parallelism, closure, and more. We use the magnitude of Configural Superiority Effects to measure the salience of EFs on a common scale, potentially letting us compare the strengths of various grouping principles. TBG appears promising, with our initial experiments establishing and quantifying at least three basic EFs in human vision.

Keywords: vision; Gestalts; emergent features; configural; wholes


James R. Pomerantz, Department of Psychology, MS 25, P O Box 1892, Houston, TX 77251-1892

1Mary C. Portillo is now at Department of Social Sciences, University of Houston–Downtown.

 

http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0096152311601028

 

 

Ervin Poljac, PhD

Laboratory of Experimental Psychology

University of Leuven (K.U. Leuven)

Tiensestraat 102

B-3000 Leuven

Belgium

tel. +32-16-32.61.43 (office)

fax. +32-16-32.60.99

Email: Ervin.Poljac@psy.kuleuven.be

 

donderdag 20 oktober 2011

Processing of contour closure by baboons (Papio papio).

 
 

Aan u verzonden door Sander via Google Reader:

 
 

via Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes - Vol 37, Iss 4 door Barbet, Isabelle; Fagot, Joël op 17-10-11

This study investigated the Gestalt law of closure in baboons. Using a computer-controlled self-testing procedure, we trained baboons (Papio papio) to discriminate open versus closed shapes presented on a touch screen with a two-alternative forced choice procedure. Ten baboons (OPEN + group) were trained with the open shapes serving as the positive stimulus (S+), and nine others (CLOSE + group) were trained with the closed shape serving as S+. The OPEN + group obtained higher discrimination performance than the CLOSE + group (Exp 1), but its scores declined when new line segments were added to the stimuli (Exp 2) and after smoothing the end points of the open shapes (Exp 3). The CLOSE + group was less affected by the above manipulations of local stimulus dimension, but its performance was disrupted when the collinearity end points was reduced (Exp 3). Use of a visual search task revealed that the search for an open shape among closed distractors was less attention demanding in baboons than the search for a closed shape among open ones (Exp 4). It is concluded that (1) end lines rather than closeness per se are perceptual primitives for the open versus closed discrimination in baboons, and (2) the relative emphasis on local or configural cues when processing contour closure depends on experiential factors in baboons and is thus subject to interindividual variations. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)

 
 

Dingen die u vanaf hier kunt doen:

 
 

woensdag 19 oktober 2011

Arnheim's Gestalt theory of visual balance: Examining the compositional stru...

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 

via i-Perception by Pion on 10/19/11

In Art and Visual Perception, Rudolf Arnheim, following on from Denman Ross's A Theory of Pure Design, proposed a Gestalt theory of visual composition. The current paper assesses a physicalist interpretation of Arnheim's theory, calculating an image's centre of mass (CoM). Three types of data are used: a large, representative collection of art photographs of recognised quality; croppings by experts and non-experts of photographs; and Ross and Arnheim's procedure of placing a frame around objects such as Arnheim's two black disks. Compared with control images, the CoM of art photographs was closer to an axis (horizontal, vertical, or diagonal), as was the case for photographic croppings. However, stronger, within-image, paired comparison studies, comparing art photographs with the CoM moved on or off an axis (the 'gamma-ramp study'), or comparing adjacent croppings on or off an axis (the 'spider-web study'), showed no support for the Arnheim–Ross theory. Finally, studies moving a frame around two disks, of different size, greyness, or background, did not support Arnheim's Gestalt theory. Although the detailed results did not support the Arnheim–Ross theory, several significant results were found which clearly require explanation by any adequate theory of the aesthetics of visual composition.

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

Reflections on the Parallellepipeda project. Johan Wagemans

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 

via i-Perception by Pion on 10/19/11

Experimental psycho-aesthetics---the science aimed at understanding the factors that determine aesthetic experience---is reviewed briefly as background to describe the Parallellepipeda project, a cross-over project between artists and scientists in Leuven. In particular, I sketch how it started and developed further, with close interactions between the participating artists and scientists. A few examples of specific research projects are mentioned to illustrate the kind of research questions we address and the methodological approach we have taken. We often found an effect of providing participants with additional information, a difference between novice and expert participants, and a shift with increasing experience with an artwork, in the direction of tolerating more complexity and acquiring more order from it. By establishing more connections between parts of an artwork and more associations to the artwork, it becomes a stronger Gestalt, which is more easily mastered by the viewer and leads to increased appreciation. In the final part of the paper, I extract some general lessons from the project regarding a possible new way of doing psycho-aesthetics research, which is able to solve some of the problems of traditional experimental psycho-aesthetics (eg, trade-off between experimental control and ecological validity).

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

dinsdag 11 oktober 2011

A new kind of science?

Michael Nielsen, whom I mentioned as my principle inspiration behind my recent open science talk, has just published his book on these matters. Here's a review from Nature Physics.

 
 

Naudojant „Google Reader" atsiųsta jums nuo Jonas:

 
 

per Nature Physics - Issue - nature.com science feeds autorius Timo Hannay 11.10.3

A new kind of science?

Nature Physics 7, 742 (2011). doi:10.1038/nphys2109

Author: Timo Hannay


 
 

Veiksmai, kuriuos dabar galite atlikti:

 
 

vrijdag 7 oktober 2011

workshop: "Neural Coding, Decision-Making & Integration in Time"

"This workshop brings together researchers from fields of computational neuroscience, electrophysiology & behavioral neuroscience to discuss the computational principles underlying neural coding of motor coordination & sensory integration in time with focus on adaptive behavior. Recent research highlights that uncertainty is one of the central problems in cue combination and Bayesian statistics is the systematic way of calculating with uncertainties. Importantly, statistics defines a unified language that can be used both for analyzing why the nervous system works the way it does as well as analyzing how it does so. Within the statistical framework we can formulate the problem of integration of multimodal information over time. The disciplines presented at the workshop include computational neuroscience, Bayesian statistics, psychophysics, neurophysiology, & cognitive science. Central to the format of the proposed workshop is the strong link between computational & empirical neuroscientists. A key aspect of all workshop contributions will be the development of a common theoretical language to be used across behavioral & electrophysiological experiments. The conference is organized as a workshop for 40 selected participants. The central part of the program consists of invited lectures of leading researchers from the fields of computational neuroscience, electrophysiology & behavioral neuroscience; the meeting focuses on creating a common language across these disciplines. The topics chosen, focus on the fundamental problems & limits behind existing theoretical approaches of modeling the neural processing driving human & animal behavior. Selected participants will be invited to present their work in a poster session."

http://www.frontiersin.org/events/Neural_Coding__Decision_Making/1522/All_Events

Fwd: CVNet - Helmholtz! Git yer Helmholtz!



Begin forwarded message:

From: Ben Backus <ben.backus@gmail.com>
Subject: CVNet - Helmholtz! Git yer Helmholtz!
Date: October 7, 2011 1:22:35 GMT+02:00

Dear Colleagues,

Here is the Treatise on Physiological Optics (1910, English translation 1924), free to download:

http://poseidon.sunyopt.edu/BackusLab/Helmholtz/

Ben Backus
 
--
Benjamin T. Backus, Ph.D.
Associate Professor
Graduate Center for Vision Research
SUNY College of Optometry / SUNY Eye Institute
33 West 42nd St.
New York, NY 10036
Tel. +1-212-938-1541
Fax +1-212-938-5760
http://sunyopt.edu/research/backus

woensdag 28 september 2011

Grouping and emergent features in vision: Toward a theory of basic Gestalts.

Grouping and emergent features in vision: Toward a theory of basic Gestalts.
Pomerantz, James R.; Portillo, Mary C.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Vol 37(5), Oct 2011, 1331-1349. doi: 10.1037/a0024330

Gestalt phenomena are often so powerful that mere demonstrations can confirm their existence, but Gestalts have proven hard to define and measure. Here we outline a theory of basic Gestalts (TBG) that defines Gestalts as emergent features (EFs). The logic relies on discovering wholes that are more discriminable than are the parts from which they are built. These wholes contain EFs that can act as basic features in human vision. As context is added to a visual stimulus, a hierarchy of EFs appears. Starting with a single dot and adding a second yields the first two potential EFs: the proximity (distance) and orientation (angle) between the two dots. A third dot introduces two more potential EFs: symmetry and linearity; a fourth dot produces surroundedness. This hierarchy may extend to collinearity, parallelism, closure, and more. We use the magnitude of Configural Superiority Effects to measure the salience of EFs on a common scale, potentially letting us compare the strengths of various grouping principles. TBG appears promising, with our initial experiments establishing and quantifying at least three basic EFs in human vision. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved)

----
Krista Overvliet, PhD.
 
Laboratory of  Experimental Psychology
University of Leuven
Tiensestraat 102, bus 3711
Room 00.74
3000 Leuven
Belgium
 
phone: +3216326146
skype: kristaovervliet
krista.overvliet@gmail.com
krista.overvliet@ppw.kuleuven.be
http://web.me.com/krista.overvliet

maandag 19 september 2011

Context Modulates the ERP Signature of Contour Integration

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 

via PLoS ONE Alerts: Neuroscience by Bart Machilsen et al. on 9/19/11

by Bart Machilsen, Nikolay Novitskiy, Kathleen Vancleef, Johan Wagemans

We investigated how the electrophysiological signature of contour integration is changed by the context in which a contour is embedded. Specifically, we manipulated the orientations of Gabor elements surrounding an embedded shape outline. The amplitudes of early visual components over posterior scalp regions were changed by the presence of a contour, and by the orientation of elements surrounding the contour. Differences in context type had an effect on the early P1 and N1 components, but not on the later P2 component. The presence of a contour had an effect on the N1 and P2 components, but not on the earlier P1 component. A modulatory effect of context on contour integration was observed on the N1 component. These results highlight the importance of the context in which contour integration takes place.

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

AVA Xmas meeting 2011

Link: http://www.theava.net/conf/index.php?conference=Meeting&schedConf=X2011

"We will be celebrating sixteen years of AVA Christmas Meetings with a Meeting in York on Monday 19th December.



This is a one-day meeting to be held in the Department of Psychology at the University of York



This year's invited talks will include:

1) Matteo Carandini (Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL)

2) Karl Gegenfurtner CRS Guest lecture(Giessen University)

3) Hannah Smithson 2011 Marr Medal winner (University of Oxford)



Abstracts (max length: 250 words) should be submitted by November 1st on this website:



http:/www.theava.net/conf/



Abstracts will be peer-reviewed & published in the open-access journal i-Perception (so long as presenting authors attend the meeting) and should cover previously unreported research on any aspect of vision in humans, animals and machines."

zondag 18 september 2011

Depth. Jan Koenderink, Andrea J van Doorn, Johan Wagemans

 
 

Sent to you by Frouke via Google Reader:

 
 

via i-Perception by Pion on 9/15/11

Depth is the feeling of remoteness, or separateness, that accompanies awareness in human modalities like vision and audition. In specific cases depths can be graded on an ordinal scale, or even measured quantitatively on an interval scale. In the case of pictorial vision this is complicated by the fact that human observers often appear to apply mental transformations that involve depths in distinct visual directions. This implies that a comparison of empirically determined depths between observers involves pictorial space as an integral entity, whereas comparing pictorial depths as such is meaningless. We describe the formal structure of pictorial space purely in the phenomenological domain, without taking recourse to the theories of optics which properly apply to physical space, a distinct ontological domain. We introduce a number of general ways to design and implement methods of geodesy in pictorial space, and discuss some basic problems associated with such measurements. The paper mainly deals with conceptual issues.

 
 

Things you can do from here:

 
 

dinsdag 23 augustus 2011

Believing Is Seeing: Troland Winner Peers Into Perceptual and Conceptual Lea...

 
 

Aan u verzonden door Sander via Google Reader:

 
 


Human concept learning clearly depends upon perception. Our concept of "gerbil" is built out of perceptual features such as "furry," "small," and "four-legged." However, recent research has found that the dependency works both ways. Perception not only influences, but is influenced by, the concepts that we learn. Our laboratory has been exploring the psychological mechanisms by which concepts and perception mutually influence one another, and building computational models to show that the circle of influences is benign rather than vicious.

An initial suggestion that concept learning influences perception comes from a consideration of the differences between novices and experts. Experts in many domains, including radiologists, wine tasters, and Olympic judges, develop specialized perceptual tools for analyzing the objects in their domain of expertise. In trying to study novice/expert differences under controlled laboratory conditions, we have found that the process of learning new concepts alters perceptual judgments. In one set of experiments (Goldstone, 1994), participants first were trained to categorize simple squares into two groups, based on either their size or brightness. After this training, they made same/different judgments ("Are these two squares physically identical?") involving dimensions that were either relevant or irrelevant during categorization training. Categorizations that the participants learned in the first phase of the experiment affected their ability to make strictly physical judgments in the second phase. First, participants greatly increased their perceptual sensitivity to the dimension that was relevant during categorization, and slightly decreased their sensitivity to the irrelevant dimension. Second, the increase in sensitivity was particularly pronounced right at the boundary between the learned categories.

TWO OPPOSING MECHANISMS OF PERCEPTUAL CHANGE
In subsequent work, we have explored two additional mechanisms of perceptual change during concept learning that are, at first sight, contradictory. The first of these mechanisms, unitization, creates perceptual units that combine object components that frequently co-occur. Components that were once perceived separately become psychologically fused together. For example, we (Goldstone, 2000) gave participants extended practice learning to place a complex concatenation of doodles into Category 1, while all of the "near misses" to this pattern belonged in Category 2. All of the pieces of the Category 1 pattern must be attended to accurately categorize it, because each piece is also present in several Category 2 patterns. After 20 hours of practice with these stimuli, participants eventually can categorize the Category 1 doodle very accurately, and more quickly than would be predicted if they were explicitly combining separate pieces of information from the doodle together. Consistent with other work on perceptual unitization (Gauthier et al., 1998; Shiffrin & Lightfoot, 1997), we argue that one way of creating new perceptual building blocks is to create something like a photographic mental image for highly familiar, complex configurations. Following this analogy, just as your local camera store does not charge more money for developing photographs of crowds than pictures of a single person, once a complex mental image has been formed, it does not require any more effort to process the unit than the components from which it was built.

Figure 1 - Arbitrary dimensions can be constructed by morphing between two faces. Each of the faces in the 4 x 4 array is comprised of a value along Dimension A ranging from Face 1 to Face 2, and a value along Dimension B from Face 3 to Face 4.

The second mechanism, dimension differentiation, involves learning to isolate perceptual dimensions that were originally psychologically fused together. For example, saturation and brightness are fused aspects of color for most people, in the same way that "heat" and "temperature" are fused together in most people's minds before they take a course in physics. However, if only one of these fused dimensions is relevant for a categorization, people can become selectively sensitized to that one dimension (Goldstone, 1994). Furthermore, Goldstone and Steyvers (2001) have argued that genuinely arbitrary dimensions can become isolated from each other. Their subjects first learned to group the 16 faces shown in Figure 1 into categories that either split the faces horizontally or vertically into two groups with eight faces each. The faces varied along arbitrary dimensions that were created by morphing between randomly paired faces. Dimension A was formed by gradually blending from Face 1 to Face 2, while Dimension B was formed by gradually blending from Face 3 to Face 4. Each of the remaining faces is defined half by its value on Dimension A and half by its value on Dimension B. Results showed that 1) people could easily learn either horizontal or vertical categorization rules; 2) once a categorization was learned, participants could effectively and automatically ignore variation along the irrelevant dimension; 3) the category-relevant dimension became especially sensitized when participants were given a transfer same/different perceptual judgment task; and 4) there was positive transfer between categorization rules that presumed the same organization of faces into perceptual dimensions and negative transfer between rules that required cross-cutting, incompatible organizations. Together, these results strongly suggest that there is more to category learning than learning to selectively attend to existing dimensions. Perceptual learning also involves creating new dimensions that can then be selectively attended once created.

A COMPUTATIONAL RECONCILIATION
Unitization involves the construction of a single functional unit out of component parts. Dimension differentiation divides wholes into separate component dimensions. There is an apparent contradiction between experience creating larger "chunks" via unitization and dividing an object into more clearly delineated components via differentiation. This incongruity can be transformed into a commonality at a more abstract level. Both mechanisms depend on the requirements established by tasks and stimuli. Objects will tend to be decomposed into their parts if the parts reflect independent sources of variation, or if the parts differ in their relevancy. Parts will tend to be unitized if they co-occur frequently, with all parts indicating a similar response. Thus, unitization and differentiation are both processes that build appropriately sized representations for the tasks at hand.

We have developed computational models to show how the concept learning can lead to learning new perceptual organizations via unitization and differentiation (Goldstone et al., 2000; Goldstone, 2003). We have been drawn to neural networks that possess units that intervene between inputs and outputs and are capable of creating internal representations. For the current purposes, these intervening units can be interpreted as learned feature detectors, and represent an organism's acquired perceptual vocabulary. Just as we perceive the world through the filter of our perceptual system, so the neural network does not have direct access to the input patterns, but rather only has access to the detectors that it develops.

Figure 2 - A sample output from the CPLUS model. After being exposed to the input pictures and their categorizations, the neural network creates detectors that can be assembled, like building blocks, to recreate the inputs. The detectors are learned at the same time that they are associated to categories. (Solid lines represent excitatory connections; dashed lines represent inhibitory connections.)

The Conceptual and Perceptual Learning by Unitization and Segmentation model, or CPLUS, is given a set of pictures as inputs, and produces as output a categorization of each picture. Along the way to this categorization, the model comes up with a description of how the picture is segmented into pieces. The segmentation that CPLUS creates will tend to involve parts that 1) obey the Gestalt laws of perceptual organization by connecting object parts that have similar locations and orientations, 2) occur frequently in the set of presented pictures, and 3) are diagnostic for the categorization. For example, if the five input pictures of Figure 2 are presented to the network and labeled as belonging to Category A or Category B, then originally random detectors typically become differentiated as shown. This adaptation of the detectors reveals three important behavioral tendencies. First, detectors are created for parts that recur across the five objects, such as the lower square and upper rectangular antenna. Thus, the first input picture on the left will be represented by combining responses of the square and rectangular antenna detectors. Second, single, holistic detectors are created for objects like the rightmost input picture that do not share any large pieces with other inputs. In this way, the model can explain how the same learning process unitizes complex configurations and differentiates other inputs into pieces. Third, the detectors act as filters that lie between the actual inputs and the categories. The learned connections between the acquired detectors and the categories are shown by thick solid lines for positive connections and dashed lines for negative connections. The network learns to decompose the leftmost input picture into a square and rectangular antenna, but also learns that only the rectangular antenna is diagnostic for categorization, predicting that Category A is present and that Category B is not. Interestingly, the network builds detectors at the same time that it builds connections between the detectors and categories. The psychological implication is that our perceptual systems do not have to be set in place before we start to use them. The concepts we need can and should influence the perceptual units we create.

Figure 3 - An illustration of the creation and subsequent use of perceptual building blocks. In the first panel, a man learned about a chicken. In the second panel, the man interprets the rest of the world in terms of the chicken he has learned.
[Conceived by Robert Goldstone. Illustrated by Joe Lee]

UNITING CONCEPTS AND PERCEPTION
One of the most powerful ideas in cognitive science has been the notion that flexible cognition works by assembling a fixed set of building blocks into novel arrangements. Our work corroborates the productivity and efficiency of using building blocks to create novel arrangements, but we are also claiming that the building blocks themselves may be flexibly adaptive rather than fixed. The concepts we learn can reach down and influence the very perceptual descriptions that ground the concepts. This interactive cycle is figuratively shown in Figure 3. A person creates perceptual building blocks from their experiences in the world. Then, the person's subsequent experience of this same world is influenced by these learned building blocks.

REFERENCES
Gauthier, I., Williams, P., Tarr, M. J., & Tanaka, J. (1998). Training "greeble" experts: A framework for studying expert object recognition processes, Vision Research, 38, 2401-2428.

Goldstone, R. L. (1994a). influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 178-200.

Goldstone, R. L. (2000). Unitization during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 86-112.

Goldstone, R. L. (2003). Learning to perceive while perceiving to learn. In R. Kimchi, M. Behrmann, & C. Olson (Eds.) Perceptual organization in vision: Behavioral and neural perspectives. (pp. 233-278). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Goldstone, R. L., & Stevyers, M. (2001). The sensitization and differentiation of dimensions during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 116-139.

Goldstone, R. L., Steyvers, M., Spencer-Smith, J., & Kersten, A. (2000). Interactions between perceptual and conceptual learning. in E. Diettrich & A. B. Markman (Eds.) Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual change in humans and machines. (pp. 191-228). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Shiffrin, R. M., & Lightfoot, N. (1997). Perceptual learning of alphanumeric-like characters. In R. L. Goldstone, P. G. Schyns, & D. L. Medin (Eds.) The psychology of learning and motivation, Volume 36. (pp. 45-82). San Diego: Academic Press.

ABOUT THE TROLAND RESEARCH AWARD
The Troland Award is given each year to two young investigators (age 40 or younger) to recognize unusual achievement and further empirical research in psychology regarding the relationships of consciousness and the physical world. Funds are to be used by the awardee to support his or her research within the broad spectrum of experimental psychology, including, for example, the topics of sensation, perception, motivation, emotion, learning, memory, cognition, language, and action. For both awards, preference will be given to experimental work taking a quantitative or other formal approach. For more information, please visit www.nationalacademies.org.

Recipients
1984 Edward N. Pugh
1985 Keith D. White
1986 Roger Ratcliff
1987 Laurence T. Maloney & Brian A. Wandell
1988 Eric I. Knudsen
1989 John T. Cacioppo
1990 Robert Desimone
1991 Daniel L. Schacter
1992 Martha Farah
1993 Steven Pinker
1994 Donald D. Hoffman & David G. Lavond
1995 Michael S. Fanselow & Robert M. Nosofsky
1996 Joseph E. Steinmetz & Steven G. Yantis
1997 Richard Ivry & Keith R. Kluender
1998 Virginia M. Richards & Jeffrey D. Schall
1999 Nancy G. Kanwisher & Harold E. Pashler
2000 Elizabeth Gould & Earl K. Miller
2001 Steven J. Luck & Karen Wynn
2002 David J. Heeger & John K. Kruschke
2003 David C. Plaut & Michael J. Tarr
2004 Robert L. Goldstone & Wendy A. Suzuki

 
 

Dingen die u vanaf hier kunt doen:

 
 

donderdag 18 augustus 2011

The same binding in contour integration and crowding

http://www.journalofvision.org/content/11/8/10.short

 

The same binding in contour integration and crowding

1.  Ramakrishna Chakravarthi Author Home PageSend Mail to Author1,2 and

2.  Denis G. Pelli Author Home PageSend Mail to Author3

+Author Affiliations

1.       1Université de Toulouse, CerCo, UPS, France
2.       2CNRS, UMR 5549, Faculté de Médecine de Rangueil, Toulouse, France
3.       3Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA

Abstract

Binding of features helps object recognition in contour integration but hinders it in crowding. In contour integration, aligned adjacent objects group together to form a path. In crowding, flanking objects make the target unidentifiable. However, to date, the two tasks have only been studied separately. K. A. May and R. F. Hess (2007) suggested that the same binding mediates both tasks. To test this idea, we ask observers to perform two different tasks with the same stimulus. We present oriented grating patches that form a “snake letter” in the periphery. Observers report either the identity of the whole letter (contour integration task) or the phase of one of the grating patches (crowding task). We manipulate the strength of binding between gratings by varying the alignment between them, i.e., the Gestalt goodness of continuation, measured as “wiggle.” We find that better alignment strengthens binding, which improves contour integration and worsens crowding. Observers show equal sensitivity to alignment in these two very different tasks, suggesting that the same binding mechanism underlies both phenomena. It has been claimed that grouping among flankers reduces their crowding of the target. Instead, we find that these published cases of weak crowding are due to weak binding resulting from target–flanker misalignment. We conclude that crowding is mediated solely by the grouping of flankers with the target and is independent of grouping among flankers.

 

 

-----------------------------------------------------------

Lee de-Wit (PhD), Post-Doctoral Researcher

Gestalt ReVision Project, University of Leuven.

https://sites.google.com/site/leehdewit/

http://ppw.kuleuven.be/labexppsy/gestaltrevision/

lee.dewit@psy.kuleuven.be, ++32 16 326 143

-----------------------------------------------------------